: MISCELLANEOUS
PASSAGE
I
The persistent patterns in the way nations
fight reflect their cultural and historical traditions and deeply rooted
attitudes that collectively make up their strategic culture. These patterns
provide insights that go beyond what can be learnt just by comparing armaments
and divisions. In the Vietnam War, the strategic tradition of the United States
called for forcing the enemy to fight a massed battle in an open area, where
superior American weapons would prevail. The United States was trying to
re-fight World War II in the jungles of Southeast Asia ,
against an enemy with no intention of doing so.
Some British military historians describe
the Asian way of war as one of indirect attacks, avoiding frontal attacks meant
to overpower an opponent. This traces back to Asian history and geography: the
great distances and harsh terrain have often made it difficult to execute the
sort of open field clashes allowed by the flat terrain and relatively compact
size of Europe . A very different strategic
tradition arose in Asia .
The bow and arrow were metaphors for an
Eastern way of war. By its nature, the arrow is an indirect weapon. Fired from
a distance of hundreds of yards, it does not necessitate physical contact with
the enemy. Thus, it can be fired from hidden positions. When fired from behind
a ridge, the barrage seems to come out of nowhere, taking the enemy by
surprise. The tradition of this kind of fighting is captured in the classical
strategic writings of the East. The 2,000 years’ worth of Chinese writings on
war constitutes the most subtle writings on the subject in any language. Not
until Clausewitz, did the West produce a strategic theorist to match the
sophistication of Sun-tzu, whose Art of
War was written 2,300 years earlier.
In Sun-tzu and other Chinese writings, the
highest achievement of arms is to defeat an adversary without fighting. He
wrote: “To win one hundred victories in one hundred battles is not the acme of
skill. To subdue the enemy without fighting is the supreme excellence.” Actual
combat is just one among many means towards the goal of subduing an adversary. War
contains too many surprises to be a first resort. It can lead to ruinous
losses, as has been seen time and again. It can have the unwanted effect of
inspiring heroic efforts in an enemy, as the United States learned in Vietnam,
and as the Japanese found out after Pearl Harbor.
Aware of the uncertainties of a military
campaign, Sun-tzu advocated war only after most thorough preparations. Even
then it should be quick and clean. Ideally, the army is just an instrument to
deal the final blow to an enemy already weakened by isolation, poor morale, and
disunity. Ever since Sun-tzu, the Chinese have been seen as masters of subtlety
who take measured actions to manipulate an adversary without his knowledge. The
dividing line between war and peace can be obscure. Low level violence often is
the backdrop to a larger strategic campaign. The unwitting victim, focused on
the day-to-day events, never realizes what’s happening to him until it’s too
late. History holds many examples. The Viet Cong lured French and U.S. infantry
deep into the jungle, weakening their morale over several years. The mobile
army of the United States
was designed to fight on the plains of Europe ,
where it could quickly move unhindered from one spot to the next. The jungle
did more than make quick movement impossible; broken down smaller units and
scattered in isolated bases, US forces were deprived of the feeling of support
and protection that ordinarily comes from being part of a big army.
The isolation of U.S. troops in Vietnam was not
just a logistical detail, something that could be overcome by, for instance,
bringing in reinforcements by helicopter. In a big army reinforcements are
readily available. It was Napoleon who realized the extraordinary effects on
morale that come from being part of a larger formation. Just the knowledge of
it lowers the soldier’s fear and increases his aggressiveness. In the jungle
and on isolated bases, this feeling was removed. The thick vegetation slowed
down the reinforcements and made it difficult to find stranded units. Soldiers
felt they were on their own.
More important, by altering the way the war
was fought, the Viet Cong stripped the United States of its belief in the
inevitability of victory, as it had done to the French before them. Morale was
high when these armies first went to Vietnam . Only after many years of
debilitating and demoralizing fighting did Hanoi launch its decisive attacks, at
Dienbienphu in 1954 and against Saigon in
1975. It should be recalled that in the final push to victory the North
Vietnamese abandoned their jungle guerrilla tactics completely, committing
their entire army of twenty divisions to pushing the South Vietnamese into
collapse. This final battle, with the enemy’s army all in one place, was the
one that the United States
had desperately wanted to fight in 1965. When it did come out into the open in
1975, Washington
had already withdrawn its forces and there was no possibility of
re-intervention.
The Japanese early in World War II used a
modern form of the indirect attack, one that relied on stealth and surprise for
its effect. At Pearl Harbor , in the Philippines ,
and in Southeast Asia , stealth and surprise
were attained by sailing under radio silence so that the navy’s movements could
not be tracked. Moving troops aboard ships into Southeast
Asia made it appear that the Japanese army was also “invisible.”
Attacks against Hawaii
and Singapore
seemed, to the American and British defenders, to come from nowhere. In Indonesia and
the Philippines
the Japanese attack was even faster than the German blitz against France in the
West.
The greatest military surprises in American
history have all been in Asia . Surely there is
something going on here beyond the purely technical difficulties of detecting
enemy movements. Pearl Harbor , the Chinese
intervention in Korea ,
the Tet offensive in Vietnam
all came out of a tradition of surprise and stealth. U.S. technical intelligence—the
location of enemy units and their movements—was greatly improved after each
surprise, but with no noticeable improvement in the American ability to foresee
or prepare what would happen next. There is a cultural divide here, not a
technical one. Even when it was possible to track an army with intelligence
satellites, as when Iraq
invaded Kuwait
or when Syria
and Egypt
attacked Israel ,
surprise was achieved. The United
States was stunned by Iraq ’s attack
on Kuwait
even though it had satellite pictures of Iraqi troops massing at the border.
The exception that proves the point that
cultural differences obscure the West’s understanding of Asian behavior was the
Soviet Union ’s 1979 invasion of Afghanistan .
This was fully anticipated and understood in advance. There was no surprise
because the United States
understood Moscow ’s
world view and thinking. It could anticipate Soviet action almost as well as
the Soviets themselves, because the Soviet Union
was really a Western country.
The difference between the Eastern and the
Western way of war is striking. The west’s great strategic writer, Clausewitz,
linked war with politics, as did Sun-tzu. Both were opponents of militarism, of
turning war over to the generals. But there all similarity ends. Clausewitz
wrote that the way to achieve a larger political purpose is through destruction
of the enemy’s army. After observing Napoleon conquer Europe
by smashing enemy armies to bits, Clausewitz made his famous remark in On War (1932) that combat is the
continuation of politics by violent means. Morale and unity are important, but
they should be harnessed for the ultimate battle. If the Eastern way of war is
embodied by the stealthy archer, the metaphorical Western counterpart is the
swordsman charging forward, seeking a decisive showdown, eager to administer
the blow that will obliterate the enemy once and for all. In this view, war
proceeds along a fixed course and occupies a finite extent of time, like a play
in three acts with a beginning, a middle, and an end. The end, the final scene,
decides the issue for good.
When things don’t work out quite this way,
the Western military mind feels tremendous frustration. Sun-tzu’s great
disciples, Mao Zedong and Ho Chi Minh, are respected in Asia
for their clever use of indirection and deception to achieve an advantage over
stronger adversaries. But in the West their approach is seen as underhand and
devious. To the American strategic mind, the Viet Cong guerrilla did not fight
fairly. He should have come out into the open and fought like a man, instead of
hiding in the jungle and sneaking around like a cat in the night.
1. According to the author, the main reason
for the U.S.
losing the Vietnam war was
1. the Vietnamese understood
the local terrain better.
2. the lack of
support for the war from the American people.
3.
the failure of the U.S.
to mobilize its military strength.
4. their
inability to fight a war on terms other than those they understood well.
2. Which of the following statements does not
describe the ‘Asian’ way of war?
1. Indirect attacks without frontal attacks.
2. The swordsman charging forward to
obliterate the enemy once and for all.
3. Manipulation of an adversary without his
knowledge.
4. Subduing an enemy without fighting.
3.
Which of the following is not one of Sun-tzu’s ideas?
1. Actual combat is the principal means of
subduing an adversary.
2. War should be undertaken only after
thorough preparation.
3. War is linked to politics.
4. War should not be left to the generals
alone.
4.
The difference in the concepts of war of Clausewitz and Sun-tzu is best
characterized by
1. Clausewitz’s support for militarism as
against Sun-tzu’s opposition to it.
2. their relative degrees of sophistication.
3. their attitude to guerrilla warfare.
4. their differing conceptions of the
structure, time and sequence of a war.
5.
To the Americans, the approach of the Viet Cong seemed devious because
1. the Viet Cong did not fight like men out in
the open.
2. the Viet Cong allied with America ’s
enemies.
3. the Viet Cong took strategic advice from
Mao Zedong.
4. the Viet Cong used bows and arrows rather
than conventional weapons.
6.
According to the author, the greatest military surprises in American
history have been in Asia because
1. The Americans failed to implement their
military strategies many miles away from their own country.
2. The Americans were unable to use their
technologies like intelligence satellites effectively to detect enemy
movements.
3. The Americans failed to understand the
Asian culture of war that was based on stealth and surprise.
4. Clausewitz is inferior to Sun-tzu.
PASSAGE II
Studies of the factors governing reading development
in young children have achieved a remarkable degree of consensus over the past
two decades. This consensus concerns the causal role of phonological skills in
young children’s reading progress. Children, who have good phonological skills
or good ‘phonological awareness’, become good readers and good spellers.
Children with poor phonological skills progress more poorly. In particular,
those who have a specific phonological deficit are likely to be classified as
dyslexic by the time that they are 9 or 10 years old.
Phonological skills in young children can be measured
at a number of different levels. The term phonological
awareness is a global one, and refers to a deficit in recognising smaller
units of sound within spoken words. Developmental work has shown that this
deficit can be at the level of syllables, of onsets and rimes, or of phonemes.
For example, a 4-year old child might have difficulty in recognising that a
word like valentine has three
syllables, suggesting a lack of syllabic awareness.
A 5-year old might have difficulty in recognising that the odd word out in the
set of words fan, cat, hat, mat is fan. This task requires an awareness of
the sub-syllabic units of the onset and
the rime. The onset corresponds to
any initial consonants in a syllable, and the rime corresponds to the vowel and
to any following consonants. Rimes correspond to rhyme in single-syllable
words, and so the rime in fan differs
from the rime in cat, hat, and mat. In longer words, rime and rhyme
may differ. The onsets in val:en:tine are
/v/ and /t/, and the rimes correspond to the spelling patterns ‘al’, ‘en’, and ‘ine’.
A 6-year old might have difficulty in recognising that
plea and pray begin with the same initial sound. This is a phonemic judgement. Although the initial
phoneme /p/ is shared between the two
words, in plea it is part of the
onset ‘pl’, and in pray it is part of the onset ‘pr’. Until children can segment the
onset (or the rime), such phonemic judgements are difficult for them to make.
In fact, a recent survey of different developmental studies has shown that the
different levels of phonological awareness appear to emerge sequentially. The
awareness of syllables, onsets, and rimes appears to emerge at around the ages
of 3 and 4, long before most children go to school. The awareness of phonemes,
on the other hand, usually emerges at around the age of 5 or 6, when children
have been taught to read for about a year. An awareness of onsets and rimes
thus appears to be a precursor of reading, whereas an awareness of phonemes at
every serial position in a word only appears to develop as reading is taught.
The onset-rime and phonemic levels of phonological structure, however, are not
distinct. Many onsets in English are single phonemes, and so are some rimes
(e.g., sea, go, zoo).
The early awareness of onsets and rimes is supported
by studies that have compared the development of phonological awareness of
onsets, rimes, and phonemes in the same subjects using the same phonological
awareness tasks. For example, a study by Treiman and Zudowski used a
same/different judgement task based on the beginning or the end sounds of
words. In the beginning sound task, the words either began with the same onset,
as in plea and plank, or shared only the initial phoneme, as in plea and pray. In the end-sound task, the words either shared the entire
rime, as in spit and wit, or shared only the final phoneme,
as in rat and wit. Treiman and Zudowski showed that 4-year and 5-year old
children found the onset-rime version of the same/different task significantly
easier than the version based on phonemes. Only the 6-year-olds, who had been
learning to read for about a year, were able to perform both versions of the
tasks with an equal level of success.
7. From the
following statements, pick out the true statement according to the passage:
1. A mono-syllabic word can have only one
onset.
2. A mono-syllabic word can have only one rhyme
but more than one rime.
3. A mono-syllabic word can have only one
phoneme.
4. All of the above.
8. Which one of the following is likely to emerge
last in the cognitive development of a child?
1. Rhyme.
2. Rime.
3. Onset.
4. Phoneme.
9. A phonological deficit in which of the
following is likely to be classified as dyslexia?
1. Phonemic judgement.
2. Onset judgement.
3. Rime judgement.
4. Any one or more of the above.
10. The Treiman and Zudowski experiment
found evidence to support the following:
1. at age 6, reading instruction helps children
perform, both, the same-different judgement task.
2. the development of onset-rime awareness
precedes the development of an awareness of phonemes.
3. at age 4-5, children find the onset-rime
version of the same/different task significantly easier.
4. the development of onset-rime awareness is a
necessary and sufficient condition for the development of an awareness of
phonemes.
11. The single-syllable words Rhyme and Rime are constituted by the exact same set of:
A. rime(s). B. onset(s). C.
rhyme(s). D. phoneme(s).
- A, B
- A, C
- A, B, C
- B, C, D
PASSAGE III
If translated into English, most of the ways
economists talk among themselves would sound plausible enough to poets,
journalists, businesspeople, and other thoughtful though noneconomical folk. Like serious talk anywhere—among boat designers
and baseball fans, say—the talk is hard to follow when one has not made a habit
of listening to it for a while. The culture of the conversation makes the words
arcane. But the people in the unfamiliar conversation are not Martians.
Underneath it all (the economist’s favorite phrase) conversational habits are
similar. Economics uses mathematical models and statistical tests and market
arguments, all of which look alien to the literary eye. But looked at closely
they are not so alien. They may be seen as figures of speech—metaphors,
analogies, and appeals to authority.
Figures of speech are not mere frills. They think for
us. Someone who thinks of a market as an “invisible hand” and the organization
of work as a “production function” and his coefficients as being “significant,”
as an economist does, is giving the language a lot of responsibility. It seems
a good idea to look hard at his language.
If the economic conversation were found to depend a
lot on its verbal forms, this would not mean that economics would be not a
science, or just a matter of opinion, or some sort of confidence game. Good
poets, though not scientists, are serious thinkers about symbols; good
historians, though not scientists, are serious thinkers about data. Good
scientists also use language. What is more (though it remains to be shown) they
use the cunning of language, without particularly meaning to. The language used
is a social object, and using language is a social act. It requires cunning
(or, if you prefer, consideration), attention to the other minds present when
one speaks.
The paying of attention to one’s audience is called
“rhetoric,” a word that I later exercise hard. One uses rhetoric, of course, to
warn of a fire in a theatre or to arouse the xenophobia of the electorate. This
sort of yelling is the vulgar meaning of the word, like the president’s “heated
rhetoric” in a press conference or the “mere rhetoric” to which our enemies
stoop. Since the Greek flame was lit, though, the word has been used also in a
broader and more amiable sense, to mean the study of all the ways of
accomplishing things with language: inciting a mob to lynch the accused, to be
sure, but also persuading readers of a novel that its characters breathe, or
bringing scholars to accept the better argument and reject the worse.
The question is whether the scholar—who usually
fancies himself an announcer of “results” or a stater of “conclusions” free of
rhetoric—speaks rhetorically. Does he try to persuade? It would seem so.
Language, I just said, is not a solitary accomplishment. The scholar doesn’t
speak into the void, or to himself. He speaks to a community of voices. He
desires to be heeded, praised, published, imitated, honored, en-Nobeled. These
are the desires. The devices of language are the means.
Rhetoric is the proportioning of means to desires in
speech. Rhetoric is an economics of language, the study of how scarce means are
allocated to the insatiable desires of people to be heard. It seems on the face
of it a reasonable hypothesis that economists are like other people in being
talkers, who desire listeners when they go to the library or the laboratory as
much as when they go to the office on the polls. The purpose here is to see if
this is true, and to see if it is useful to study the rhetoric of economic
scholarship.
The subject is scholarship. It is not economy, or the
adequacy of economic theory as a description of the economy, or even mainly the
economist’s role in the economy. The subject is the conversation economists
have among themselves, for purposes of persuading each other that the interest
elasticity of demand for investment is zero or that the money supply is
controlled by the Federal Reserve.
Unfortunately, though, the conclusions are of more
than academic interest. The conversations of classicists or of astronomers
rarely affect the lives of other people. Those of economists do so on a large
scale. A well known joke describes a May Day parade through Red
Square with the usual mass of soldiers, guided missiles, and
rocket launchers. At last come rank upon rank of people in gray business suits.
A bystander asks, “Who are those?” “Aha!” comes the reply, “those are
economists: you have no idea what damage they can do!” Their conversations do
it.
12. According to the passage, which of the
following is the best set of reasons for which one needs to “look hard” at the economist’s language?
a. Economists accomplish a great deal through
their language.
b. Economics is an opinion-based subject.
c.
Economics
has a great impact on other’s lives.
d. Economics is damaging.
1. a and
b 2. c and d 3. a and c 4. b and d
13.
In the light of the definition of rhetoric given in the passage, which
of the following will have the least element of rhetoric?
1. An election speech.
2. An advertisement jingle.
3. Dialogues in a play.
4. Commands given by army officers.
14. As used in the passage, which of the
following is the closest meaning to the statement “The culture of the
conversation makes the words arcane”?
1. Economists belong to a different culture.
2. Only mathematicians can understand
economists.
3. Economists tend to use terms unfamiliar to
the lay person, but depend on familiar linguistic forms.
4. Economists use similes and adjectives in
their analysis.
15. As used
in the passage, which of the following is the closest alternative to the word
‘arcane’?
1.
Mysterious 2. Secret 3. Covert 4. Perfidious
16.
Based on your understanding of the passage, which of the following
conclusions would you agree with?
1. The geocentric and the heliocentric views
of the solar system are equally tenable.
2. The heliocentric view is superior because
of better rhetoric.
3. Both views use rhetoric to persuade.
4. Scientists should not use rhetoric.
PASSAGE IV
At the heart of the enormous boom in wine consumption
that has taken place in the English-speaking world over the last two decades or
so is a fascinating, happy paradox. In the days when wine was exclusively the
preserve of a narrow cultural elite, bought either at auctions or from
gentlemen wine merchants in wing collars and bow-ties, to be stored in rambling
cellars and decanted to order by one’s butler, the ordinary drinker didn’t get
a look-in. Wine was considered a highly technical subject, in which anybody
without the necessary ability could only fall flat on his or her face in
embarrassment. It wasn’t just that you needed a refined aesthetic sensibility
for the stuff if it wasn’t to be hopelessly wasted on you. It required an
intimate knowledge of what came from where, and what it was supposed to taste
like.
Those were times, however, when wine appreciation essentially
meant a familiarity with the great French classics, with perhaps a smattering
of other wines—like sherry and port. That was what the wine trade dealt in.
These days, wine is bought daily in supermarkets and high-street chains to be
consumed that evening, hardly anybody has a cellar to store it in and most
don’t even possess a decanter. Above all, the wines of literally dozens of
countries are available on the market. When a supermarket offers its customers
a couple of fruity little numbers from Brazil , we scarcely raise an
eyebrow.
It seems, in other words, that the commercial jungle
that wine has now become has not in the slightest deterred people from plunging
adventurously into the thickets in order to taste and see. Consumers are no
longer intimidated by the thought of needing to know their Pouilly-Fuisse, just
at the very moment when there is more to know than ever before.
The reason for this new mood of confidence is not hard
to find. It is on every wine label from Australia , New Zealand , South Africa
and the United States :
the name of the grape from which the wine is made. At one time that might have
sounded like a fairly technical approach in itself. Why should native
English-speakers know what Cabernet Sauvignon or Chardonnay were? The answer
lies in the popularity that wines made from those grape varieties now enjoy.
Consumers effectively recognize them as brand names, and have acquired a basic
lexicon of wine that can serve them even when confronted with those Brazilian
upstarts.
In the wine heartlands of France, they are scared to
death of that trend—not because they think their wine isn’t as good as the best
from California or South Australia (What French winemaker will ever admit
that?) but because they don’t traditionally call their wines Cabernet Sauvignon
or Chardonnay. They call them Chateau Ducru-Beaucaillou or Corton-Charlemagne,
and they aren’t about to change. Some areas, in the middle of southern France , have
now produced a generation of growers using the varietal names on their labels
and are tempting consumers back to French wine. It will be an uphill struggle,
but there is probably no other way if France is to avoid simply becoming
a specialty source of old-fashioned wines for old-fashioned connoisseurs.
Wine consumption was also given a significant boost in
the early 1990s by the work of Dr. Serge Renaud, who has spent many years
investigating the reasons for the uncannily low incidence of coronary heart
disease in the south of France .
One of his major findings is that the fat-derived cholesterol that builds up in
the arteries and can eventually lead to heart trouble can be dispersed by the
tannins in wine. Tannin is derived from the skins of grapes, and is therefore
present in higher levels in red wines, because they have to be infused with
their skins to attain the red colour. That news caused a huge upsurge in red
wine consumption in the United
States . It has not been accorded the
prominence it deserves in the UK ,
largely because the medical profession still sees all alcohol as a menace to
health, and is constantly calling for it to be made prohibitively expensive.
Certainly the manufacturers of anticoagulant drugs might have something to lose
if we all got the message that we would do just as well by our hearts by taking
half a bottle of red wine every day!
17. Which one of the following, if true, would
provide most support for Dr. Renaud’s findings about the effect of tannins?
1.
A
survey showed that film celebrities based in France have a low incidence of
coronary heart disease.
2.
Measurements
carried out in southern France
showed red wine drinkers had significantly higher levels of coronary heart
incidence than white wine drinkers did.
3.
Data
showed a positive association between sales of red wine and incidence of
coronary heart disease.
4.
Long-term
surveys in southern France
showed that the incidence of coronary heart disease was significantly lower in
red wine drinkers than in those who did not drink red wine.
18. Which one of the following CANNOT be
reasonably attributed to the labelling strategy followed by wine producers in
English-speaking countries?
1.
Consumers buy wines on the basis of their familiarity with a grape
variety’s name.
2. Even ordinary customers now have more access
to technical knowledge about wine.
3. Consumers are able to appreciate better
quality wines.
4. Some non-English speaking countries like Brazil indicate
grape variety names on their labels.
19.
The tone that the author uses while asking “What French winemaker will
ever admit that?” is best described as
1.
caustic. 2. satirical. 3.
critical. 4. hypocritical.
20.
The development which has created fear among winemakers in the wine
heartlands of France
is the
1.
tendency not to name wines after the grape varieties that are used in
the wines.
2. ‘education’ that consumers have derived from
wine labels from English-speaking countries.
3. new generation of local winegrowers who use
labels that show names of grape varieties.
4. ability of consumers to understand a wine’s
qualities when confronted with “Brazilian upstarts”.
21. What according to the author should the
French do to avoid becoming a producer of merely old-fashioned wines?
1.
Follow the labelling strategy of the English-speaking countries.
2. Give their wines English names.
3. Introduce fruity wines as Brazil has
done.
4. Produce the wines that have become popular
in the English-speaking world.
PASSAGE V
Pure love of learning, of course, was a less compelling
motive for those who became educated for careers other than teaching. Students
of law in particular had a reputation for being materialistic careerists in an
age when law was becoming known as “the lucrative science” and its successful
practice the best means for rapid advancement in the government of both church
and state. Medicine too had its profit-making attractions. Those who did not go
on to law or medicine could, if they had been well trained in the arts, gain
positions at royal courts or rise in the clergy. Eloquent testimony to the
profit motive behind much of twelfth-century education was the lament of a
student of Abelard around 1150 that “Christians educate their sons...for gain,
in order that the one brother, if he be a clerk, may help his father and mother
and his other brothers, saying that a clerk will have no heir and whatever he
has will be ours and the other brothers’.” With the opening of positions in
law, government, and the church, education became a means for advancement not
only in income but also in status. Most who were educated were wealthy, but in
the twelfth century, more often than before, many were not and were able to
rise through the ranks by means of their education. The most familiar examples
are Thomas Becket, who rose from a humble background to become chancellor of England and
archbishop of Canterbury ,
and John of Salisbury, who was born a “plebian” but because of his reputation
for learning died as bishop of Chartres .
The instances of Becket and John of Salisbury bring us
to the most difficult question concerning twelfth-century education: To what
degree was it still a clerical preserve? Despite the fact that throughout the
twelfth century the clergy had a monopoly of instruction, one of the
outstanding medievalists of our day, R.W. Southern, refers with good reason to
the institutions staffed by the clergy as “secular schools.” How can we make
sense out of the paradox that twelfth-century schools were clerical and yet
“secular”?
Let us look at the clerical side first. Not only were
all twelfth-century teachers except professionals and craftsmen in church
orders, but in northern Europe students in
schools had clerical status and looked like priests. Not that all really were
priests, but by virtue of being students all were awarded the legal privileges
accorded to the clergy. Furthermore, the large majority of twelfth-century
students, outside of the possible exception of Italy , if not already priests
became so after studies were finished. For these reasons, the term “cleric” was
often used to denote a man who was literate and the term “layman” one who was
illiterate. The English word for cleric, clerk, continued for a long time to be
a synonym for student or for a man who could write, while the French word clerc even today has the connotation of
intellectual.
Despite all this, twelfth-century education was taking
on many secular qualities in its environment, goals, and curriculum. Student
life obviously became more secular when it moved from the monasteries into the
bustling towns. Most students wandered from town to town in search not only of
good masters but also of worldly excitement, and as the twelfth century
progressed they found the best of each in Paris .
More important than environment was the fact that most students, even though
they entered the clergy, had secular goals. Theology was recognized as the
“queen of the sciences,” but very few went on to it. Instead they used their
study of the liberal arts as a preparation for law, medicine, government
service, or advancement in the ecclesiastical hierarchy. This being so, the
curriculum of the liberal arts became more sophisticated and more divorced from
religion. Teaching was still almost exclusively in Latin, and the first book
most often read was the Psalter, but further education was no longer similar to
that of a choir school. In particular, the discipline of rhetoric was
transformed from a linguistic study into instruction in how to compose letters
and documents; there was a new stress on logic; and in all the liberal arts and
philosophy texts more advanced than those known in the early Middle Ages were
introduced.
Along with the rise of logic came the translation of
Greek and Arabic philosophical and scientific works. Most important was the
translation of almost all the writings of Aristotle, as well as his
sophisticated Arabic commentators, which helped to bring about an intellectual
revolution based on Greek rationalism. On a more prosaic level, contact with
Arabs resulted in the introduction in the twelfth century of the Arabic numeral
system and the concept of zero. Though most westerners first resisted this and
made crude jokes about the zero as an ambitious number “that counts for nothing
and yet wants to be counted,” the system steadily made its inroads first in Italy and then
throughout Europe , thereby vastly simplifying
the arts of computation and record keeping.
22. According to the passage, what led to the
secularization of the curriculum of the liberal arts in the twelfth century?
1.
It was divorced from religion and its influences.
2. Students used it mainly as a base for
studying law and medicine.
3. Teaching could no longer be conducted
exclusively in Latin.
4. Arabic was introduced into the curriculum.
23.
According to the author, in the twelfth century, individuals were
motivated to get higher education because it:
1.
was a means for material advancement and higher status.
2. gave people with wealth an opportunity to
learn.
3. offered a coveted place for those with a
love of learning.
4. directly added to the income levels of
people.
24. According to the passage, twelfth century
schools were clerical and yet secular because:
1.
many teachers were craftsmen and professionals who did not form part of
the church.
2. while the students had the legal privileges
accorded to the clergy and looked like priests, not all were really priests.
3. the term “cleric” denoted a literate
individual rather than a strict association with the church.
4. though the clergy had a monopoly in
education, the environment, objectives and curriculum in the schools were
becoming secular.
25. What does the sentence “Christians educate
their sons...will be ours and the other brothers’ ” imply?
1.
The Christian family was a close-knit unit in the twelfth century.
2. Christians educated their sons not so much
for the love of learning as for material gain.
3. Christians believed very strongly in
educating their sons in the Church.
4.
The relationship between Christian parents and their sons was
exploitative in the twelfth century.
26.
According to the passage, which of the following is the most noteworthy
trend in education in twelfth-century Europe ?
1.
Secularization of education.
2. Flowering of theology as the queen of the sciences.
3. Wealthy people increasingly turning to
education.
4. Rise of the clergy’s influence on the
curriculum.
PASSAGE VI
Fifty feet away three male lions lay by the road. They
didn’t appear to have a hair on their heads. Noting the color of their noses
(leonine noses darken as they age, from pink to black), Craig estimated that
they were six years old—young adults. “This is wonderful!’ he said, after
staring at them for several moments. “This is what we came to see. They really
are maneless.” Craig, a professor at the University of Minnesota ,
is arguably the leading expert on the majestic Serengeti lion, whose head is
mantled in long, thick hair. He and Peyton West, a doctoral student who has
been working with him in Tanzania ,
had never seen the Tsavo lions that live some 200 miles east of the Serengeti.
The scientists had partly suspected that the maneless males were adolescents
mistaken for adults by amateur observers. Now they knew better.
The Tsavo research expedition was mostly Peyton’s
show. She had spent several years in Tanzania , compiling the data she
needed to answer a question that ought to have been answered long ago: Why do
lions have manes? It’s the only cat, wild or domestic, that displays such
ornamentation. In Tsavo she was attacking the riddle from the opposite angle.
Why do its lions not have manes? (Some “maneless” lions in Tsavo East do have
partial manes, but they rarely attain the regal glory of the Serengeti lions’.)
Does environmental adaptation account for the trait? Are the lions of Tsavo, as
some people believe, a distinct subspecies of their Serengeti cousins?
The Serengeti lions have been under continuous
observation for more than 35 years, beginning with George Schaller’s pioneering
work in the 1960s. But the lions in Tsavo ,
Kenya ’s oldest
and largest protected ecosystem, have hardly been studied. Consequently,
legends have grown up around them. Not only do they look different, according
to the myths, they behave differently,
displaying greater cunning and aggressiveness. “Remember too,” Kenya: The Rough Guide warns, “Tsavo’s
lions have a reputation of ferocity.” Their fearsome image became well-known in
1898, when two males stalled construction of what is now Kenya Railways by
allegedly killing and eating 135 Indian and African laborers. A British Army
officer in charge of building a railroad bridge over the Tsavo River ,
Lt. Col. J. H. Patterson, spent nine months pursuing the pair before he brought
them to bay and killed them. Stuffed and mounted, they now glare at visitors to
the Field Museum in Chicago . Patterson’s account of the leonine
reign of terror, The Man-Eaters of Tsavo,
was an international best-seller when published in 1907. Still in print, the
book has made Tsavo’s lions notorious. That annoys some scientists. “People
don’t want to give up on mythology,” Dennis King told me one day. The zoologist
has been working in Tsavo off and on for four years. “I am so sick of this
man-eater business. Patterson made a helluva lot of money off that story, but
Tsavo’s lions are no more likely to turn man-eater than lions from elsewhere.”
But tales of their savagery and wiliness don’t all
come from sensationalist authors looking to make a buck. Tsavo lions are
generally larger than lions elsewhere, enabling them to take down the
predominant prey animal in Tsavo, the Cape buffalo, one of the strongest, most
aggressive animals on Earth. The buffalo don’t give up easily: They often kill
or severely injure an attacking lion, and a wounded lion might be more likely
to turn to cattle and humans for food.
And other prey is less abundant in Tsavo than in other
traditional lion haunts. A hungry lion is more likely to attack humans. Safari
guides and Kenya Wildlife Service rangers tell of lions attacking Land Rovers,
raiding camps, stalking tourists. Tsavo is a tough neighborhood, they say, and
it breeds tougher lions.
But are they really tougher? And if so, is there any
connection between their manelessness and their ferocity? An intriguing
hypothesis was advanced two years ago by Gnoske and Peterhans: Tsavo lions may
be similar to the nmanned cave lions of the Pleistocene. The Serengeti variety
is among the most evolved of the species—the latest model, so to speak—while
certain morphological differences in Tsavo lions (bigger bodies, smaller
skulls, and maybe even lack of a mane) suggest that they are closer to the
primitive ancestor of all lions. Craig and Peyton had serious doubts about the
idea, but admitted that Tsavo lions pose a mystery to science.
27. The book
Man-Eaters of Tsavo annoys some
scientists because
1. it revealed that Tsavo lions are ferocious.
2. Patterson made a helluva lot of money from
the book by sensationalism.
3. it perpetuated the bad name Tsavo lions
had.
4. it narrated how two male lions were killed.
28.
According to the passage, which of the following has NOT contributed to
the popular image of Tsavo lions as savage creatures?
1.
Tsavo
lions have been observed to bring down one of the strongest and most aggressive
animals—
the Cape buffalo.
2.
In
contrast to the situation in traditional lion haunts, scarcity of non-buffalo
prey in the Tsavo makes the Tsavo lions more aggressive.
3.
The
Tsavo lion is considered to be less evolved than the Serengeti variety.
4.
Tsavo
lions have been observed to attack vehicles as well as humans.
29. The sentence which concludes the first
paragraph, “Now they knew better”, implies that:
1.
The
two scientists were struck by wonder on seeing maneless lions for the first
time.
2.
Though
Craig was an expert on the Serengeti lion, now he also knew about the Tsavo
lions.
3.
Earlier,
Craig and West thought that amateur observers had been mistaken.
4.
Craig
was now able to confirm that darkening of the noses as lions aged applied to
Tsavo lions as well.
30.
Which of the following, if true, would weaken the hypothesis advanced by
Gnoske and Peterhans most?
1.
Craig and Peyton develop even more serious doubts about the idea that
Tsavo lions are primitive.
2.
The maneless Tsavo East lions are shown to be closer to the cave lions.
3. Pleistocene cave lions are shown to be far
less violent than believed.
4.
The morphological variations in body and skull size between the cave and
Tsavo lions are found to be insignificant.
PASSAGE
VII
A game of strategy, as currently conceived in game
theory, is a situation in which two or more “players” make choices among
available alternatives (moves). The totality of choices determines the outcomes
of the game, and it is assumed that the rank order of preferences for the
outcomes is different for different players. Thus the “interests” of the
players are generally in conflict. Whether these interests are diametrically
opposed or only partially opposed depends on the type of game.
Psychologically, most interesting situations arise
when the interests of the players are partly coincident and partly opposed,
because then one can postulate not only a conflict among the players but also
inner conflicts within the players. Each is torn between a tendency to
cooperate, so as to promote the common interests, and a tendency to compete, so
as to enhance his own individual interests.
Internal conflicts are always psychologically
interesting. What we vaguely call “interesting” psychology is in very great
measure the psychology of inner conflict. Inner conflict is also held to be an
important component of serious literature as distinguished from less serious
genres. The classical tragedy, as well as the serious novel, reveals the inner
conflict of central figures. The superficial adventure story, on the other hand,
depicts only external conflict; that is, the threats to the person with whom
the reader (or viewer) identifies stem in these stories exclusively from
external obstacles and from the adversaries who create them. On the most
primitive level this sort of external conflict is psychologically empty. In the
fisticuffs between the protagonists of good and evil, no psychological problems
are involved or, at any rate, none are depicted in juvenile representations of
conflict.
The detective story, the “adult” analogue of a
juvenile adventure tale, has at times been described as a glorification of
intellectualized conflict. However, a great deal of the interest in the plots
of these stories is sustained by withholding the unraveling of a solution to a
problem. The effort of solving the problem is in itself not a conflict if the
adversary (the unknown criminal) remains passive, like Nature, whose secrets
the scientist supposedly unravels by deduction. If the adversary actively puts
obstacles in the detective’s path toward the solution, there is genuine
conflict. But the conflict is psychologically interesting only to the extent
that it contains irrational components such as a tactical error on the
criminal’s part or the detective’s insight into some psychological quirk of the
criminal or something of this sort. Conflict conducted in a perfectly rational
manner is psychologically no more interesting than a standard Western. For
example, Tic-tac-toe, played perfectly by both players, is completely devoid of
psychological interest. Chess may be psychologically interesting but only to
the extent that it is played not quite rationally. Played completely
rationally, chess would not be different from Tic-tac-toe.
In short, a pure conflict of interest (what is called
a zero-sum game) although it offers a wealth of interesting conceptual
problems, is not interesting psychologically, except to the extent that its
conduct departs from rational norms.
31. According to the passage, which of the following
options about the application of game theory to a conflict-of-interest
situation is true?
1. Assuming that the rank order of preferences
for options is different for different players.
2. Accepting that the interests of different
players are often in conflict.
3. Not assuming that the interests are in
complete disagreement.
4. All of the above.
32. The problem
solving process of a scientist is different from that of a detective because
1. scientists study inanimate objects, while
detectives deal with living criminals or law offenders.
2. scientists study known objects, while
detectives have to deal with unknown criminals or law offenders
3. scientists study phenomena that are not
actively altered, while detectives deal with phenomena that have been
deliberately influenced to mislead.
4. scientists study psychologically interesting
phenomena, while detectives deal with “adult” analogues of juvenile adventure
tales.
33. According
to the passage, internal conflicts are psychologically more interesting than
external conflicts because
1. internal conflicts, rather than external
conflicts, form an important component of serious literature as distinguished
from less serious genres.
2. only juveniles or very few “adults” actually
experience external conflict, while internal conflict is more widely prevalent
in society.
3. in situations of internal conflict,
individuals experience a dilemma in resolving their own preferences for
different outcomes.
4. there are no threats to the reader (or
viewer) in case of external conflicts.
34. Which,
according to the author, would qualify as interesting psychology?
1. A statistician’s dilemma over choosing the
best method to solve an optimization problem.
2. A chess player’s predicament over adopting
a defensive strategy against an aggressive opponent.
3. A mountaineer’s choice of the best path to Mt. Everest
from the base camp.
4. A finance manager’s quandary over the best
way of raising money from the market.
________________________________________________________________________________________________________
ANSWER KEY
1. (4) 2.
(2) 3. (1) 4. (4) 5. (1)
6. (3) 7. (1) 8. (4) 9. (4) 10. (2)
11. (2) 12. (3) 13. (4) 14. (3) 15. (1)
16. (3) 17. (4) 18. (3) 19. (2) 20. (2)
21. (1) 22. (2) 23. (1) 24. (4) 25. (2)
26. (1) 27. (3) 28. (3) 29. (3) 30. (3)
31. (4) 32. (3) 33. (3) 34. (2)
No comments:
Post a Comment